Base de datos de no-palabras de cuatro letras para su uso en tareas experimentales de decisión léxica

  1. Alameda Bailén, José Ramón
Revista:
REMA

ISSN: 1135-6855

Año de publicación: 1997

Volumen: 2

Número: 1

Páginas: 1-49

Tipo: Artículo

Otras publicaciones en: REMA

Resumen

The no-words selection is important in lexical decision task. The similarity between no-words and words determine the recognition time to no-words, when the no-words are similarity to high frequency words the response time is highter than in the opposite case. In this paper, we present a no-words base where the similar words number, the acumulative frequency from this one, and the similar words numbers by the changing letter, is shown.

Referencias bibliográficas

  • 1. Alameda, J.R. y Cuetos, F. (1995). Diccionario de frecuencias de las unidades lingüísticas del castellano. Oviedo: Servicio de Publicaciones de la Universidad de Oviedo.
  • 2. Coltheart, M., Davelaar, E., Jonasson, J.T. y Besner, D. (1977). Access to the internal lexicon. En S. Dornic (Ed.), Attention and Performance VI. Nueva York: Academic press.
  • 3. Cuetos, F. (1990). Psicología de la lectura. Madrid: Escuela Española.
  • 4. Chambers, S.M. (1979). Letter and orden information in lexical access. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 18, 225-241.
  • 5. Forster, K.I. (1976). Accesing the mental lexicon. En R.J. Wales y E.W. Walker (Eds.), New approaches to languaje mechanism. Amsterdam: North-Holland. (Versión castellana en F. Valle, F. Cuetos, J.M. Igoa y S. del Viso (eds.): Lecturas de Psicolinguística. Madrid: Alianza Psicología, 1990).
  • 6. Forster, K.L. y Chambers, S.M. (1973). Lexical access and naming time. Journal of Verbal Leaming and Verbal Behavior, 12, 627-635.
  • 7. Fredericksen, J.R. y Kroll, J.F. (1976) Spelling and sound: Approaches to the intenal lexicon. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2, 361-379.
  • 8. Goldinger, S.D., Luce, P.A. y Pisoni, D.B. (1989). Priming lexical neighbors of spoken words: Effects of competition and inhibition. Journal of Memory and Language, 28(5), 501-518
  • 9. Grainger, J., O'Regan, J.K., Jacobs, A.M. y Segui, J. (1989). On the role of competing word units in visual word recognition: The neighborhood frequency effect. Perception and Psychophysics, 45, 189-195
  • 10. Johnston, J.C. (1978). A test of the sophisticated guessing theory of word perception. Cognitive Psichology, 10, 123-153.
  • 11. Lupker, S.J. y Colombo, L. (1994). Inhibitory affects in form priming: Evaluating a phonological competition explanation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20(2), 437-451.
  • 12. McClelland, J.L. y Rumelhart, D.E. (1988). Explorations in parallel distributed processing. Cambridge: MIT Press.
  • 13. Rubenstein, H., Garfield, L. y Millikan, J.A. (1970). Homographic entries in the internal lexicon. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 9, 487-94.
  • 14. Rumelhart, D.D. y McClelland, J.L. (1981). Interactive processing through spreading activation. En A.M. Lesgold y C.A. PerFetti (eds.) Interactive processes in reading. Hillsdale: LEA.
  • 15. Scheerer, E. (1987). Visual word recognition in German. En A. Allport, D. MacKay, W. Prinz y E. Scheerer (Eds.) Language, perception and production: Relationship between listening, speaking, reading and writing. Londres: Academic Press.
  • 16. Sebastián, N., Cuetos, F. y Carreiras, M. (en preparación). LEXESP: Creación de una base de datos informatizada del español. DGICYT: APC93-0122
  • 17. Taft, M. (1991). Reading and the mental lexicon. Hillsdale: Erlbaum.